Brain: how do we experience time?
- Psychological time (experienced and reconstructed by our brains) does not coincide with physical time (measured by our watches), but it is not completely disconnected from it.
- Scientists are now investigating the way in which neurons code the mental representation of time.
- This research calls into question the idea of an internal clock synchronised with external rhythms, which beats time and records the beats to count time.
- The promising CHRONOLOGY project aims to understand how the brain constructs a map of time.
- One of the project's intuitions is that the neural mechanisms that generate the mental mapping of time are largely common to different species.
How does our brain apprehend time? We put the question to Virginie van Wassenhove, research director at the CEA and head of the Inserm team, whose CHRONOLOGY project has just been awarded a Synergy grant from the European Research Council, worth up to €10 million over 6 years.
What is the relationship between objective time, as measured by our watches, and psychological time, as experienced and reconstructed by our brains?
Virginie van Wassenhove. It’s an ambiguous relationship! Psychological time does not coincide with physical time, it isn’t completely disconnected from it either. Let’s take a few examples: we’re perfectly capable of estimating time very accurately when we have to cross a pedestrian crossing or play ping-pong, but we lose precision if the time to be evaluated gets longer, or if we’re distracted by other stimuli.
Similarly, an hour spent in a dentist’s waiting room will seem much longer than an hour spent on a first date. Let’s go a step further. If we try to recall these two episodes years later, our temporal experience will be reversed: the wait at the dentist will seem much shorter than it actually was, and the romantic date much longer, because it was rich in emotions and micro-events to which we paid attention.
The question of psychological time is therefore complex, but fundamental, because it is on the basis of this mental representation that we project ourselves into the future and the past, develop our thinking, make short- and long-term decisions, in short, commit ourselves to life.
How did you come to work on this subject?
My interest in time dates back to the mid-2000s, during my post-doctoral work when I was working on the processing of multisensory information by the brain. The sensory stimuli associated with the same event are conveyed in different forms of energy (vibrating molecules for sound, photons for vision, etc.) and do not reach the brain at exactly the same time.
The notion of simultaneousness is therefore entirely reconstructed by the brain. But determining simultaneousness is central, because it conditions our perception: it is in fact precisely the moment when consciousness appears. So I began to take an interest in how neurons code temporality, or in other words, the mental representation of time. I spent a summer reading almost a century of literature on the subject, without finding any satisfactory answers. A new field of study was opening up for me.
What did you find unsatisfactory in the classic literature?
Very few neuroscientists asked the question of psychological time in terms of neuronal coding and mental representation. The literature seemed to be content with the model of an internal clock synchronised with external rhythms, which would beat time and record the beats to count durations. This concept emerged after the discovery of brain rhythms, and in particular the alpha rhythm, a brain wave with a period of 100 ms that can be observed in all conscious individuals. Because this rhythm is a priori very regular, the working hypothesis was that it beat the tempo of the internal clock. But the rhythmicity of certain neuronal activities is not enough to explain how the brain represents time. Coming from the field of sensory perception, this seemed obvious to me: taking the analogy of the mental processing of colour, it would be like imagining that to transmit red information, the neurons themselves would have to turn red.

The internal clock model is therefore useful, because it predicts some of our behaviour, but it didn’t seem to me to be realistic from a neurobiological point of view. Recent studies using functional neuroimaging at high temporal resolution (such as electroencephalogram [EGG] and magnetoencephalography [MEG]), including those carried out by my team, have subsequently shown that this is not the case.
What results did you obtain?
We were able to establish1 that the alpha rhythm is not constant, and this characteristic is incompatible with the very idea of a clock. So, there are some nuances to be made: yes, brain rhythms are certainly involved in temporal processing, but the story is more complicated than the internal clock model suggests. And that’s just as well… because if our conception of time were governed solely by biological clocks set to external rhythms, we would have to conclude that we are in a constant state of attentional capture and we would not be able to explain the stability of our thinking. Yet stability of thought is absolutely necessary for the emergence of consciousness. Our brain must therefore have a stable system for representing time, a time reference system that is largely immune to external temporal information. This is obvious when we consider time travel.
What do you mean by time travel?
The ability we have to imagine ourselves far into the past or project ourselves into the future. This time travel, which could be unique to human beings, requires a high degree of abstraction: we have to be able to establish a map of time in which we can move (mentally), while preserving the temporal relationships between events. The internal clock alone cannot explain this ability.
What more biologically realistic picture can we give of how the brain processes temporality?
In 2014, John O’Keefe, May-Britt Moser and Edvard I. Moser were awarded the Nobel Prize in Medicine for their decades-long work in demonstrating the existence of a ‘GPS’ within the brain. Their work showed that a multitude of neurons specific to certain characteristics of space collaborate in this GPS. Some provide a spatial metric, others code the direction of movement, others the orientation of the head, others sensory experiences. These highly sophisticated circuits support a fairly flexible representation system, enabling the animal to navigate in space and mentally map its environment. My team and I hypothesise that a similar system, highly complex and integrating diverse information, is also deployed for time. This is what we are going to explore in the CHRONOLOGY project.
This project, carried out with three other researchers, has just been awarded a Synergy grant for ‘an ambitious project at the frontiers of knowledge’. Could you tell us more about its aims and how it will be carried out?
CHRONOLOGY aims to understand how the brain maps time. One of our intuitions is that the neural mechanisms that generate the mental mapping of time are largely common to different species. Each of us will therefore be testing the representations of time in living models from different species: Brice Bathellier from the CNRS in mice, Mehrdad Jazayeri from MIT in non-human primates and myself in humans. Srdjan Ostojic, from the ENS, will build models of low-rank recurrent neural networks, developed based on biological plausibility, i.e. constrained by the architecture of the neural circuits of the three species. Thanks to the back-and-forth between these AI approaches and the behavioural experiments carried out on living models, we hope not only to identify the dynamics of the cerebral activity at the origin of our representation of time, but also to understand the causal links between the mechanisms involved.
We need this type of project, aimed first and foremost at acquiring fundamental principles that can be generalised across the animal kingdom, before tackling more applied questions such as: why are certain psychiatric or neurological disorders accompanied by temporal disorientation? The brain is the most complex system in the universe, even more complex than a star or a black hole – a star and black hole that it is itself capable of conceiving! We still have almost everything to learn about how it works.
Interview by Anne Orliac
Find out more:
- Runyun, Ş. L., van Wassenhove, V., & Balci, F. (2024), Altération de la conscience temporelle pendant la pandémie de Covid-19, Recherche psychologique, 1–11.
- Kononowicz, TW, Roger, C., & van Wassenhove, V. (2019), La métacognition temporelle comme décodage de la dynamique cérébrale auto-générée, Cortex cérébral, 29 (10), 4366–4380.
- Grabot, L., & van Wassenhove, V. (2017), L’ordre temporel comme biais psychologique, Psychological science, 28 (5), 670–678.
- Gauthier, B., & van Wassenhove, V. (2016), Le temps n’est pas l’espace : calculs de base et réseaux spécifiques au domaine pour les voyages mentaux, Journal of Neuroscience, 36 (47), 11891–11903.